Which best contrasts the difference between a mastery-approach goal and a performance-approach goal?

In order to continue enjoying our site, we ask that you confirm your identity as a human. Thank you very much for your cooperation.

  • Which best contrasts the difference between a mastery-approach goal and a performance-approach goal?
    Access through your institution

Which best contrasts the difference between a mastery-approach goal and a performance-approach goal?

Volume 61, January 2018, Pages 127-135

Which best contrasts the difference between a mastery-approach goal and a performance-approach goal?

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2017.11.017Get rights and content

Achievement goal theorists have been debating for over a decade about whether performance-approach goals (i.e., goals that aim at outperforming others) constitute a maladaptive form of motivation and hence whether it should be totally discouraged by teachers and parents (Harackiewicz et al., 2002, Midgley et al., 2001). This is because performance-approach goals, as compared to mastery-approach goals (i.e., goals focusing on attaining mastery and learning), can become a double-edged sword as the highly likely costs (such as challenge avoidance) that they inherently carry outweigh the few benefits that they might bring (e.g., higher grades) (Brophy, 2005). Although mastery-approach goals are considered more adaptive than performance-approach goals (Hulleman and Senko, 2010, Midgley et al., 2001), it should be admitted however that performance-approach goals may become attractive for many students, teachers, and parents, namely because they are positively associated with a valuable outcome: Higher grades.

Indeed, prior research has pointed out that performance-approach goals can more reliably predict academic performance than mastery-approach goals (Hulleman, Schrager, Bodmann, & Harackiewicz, 2010). Yet, as Senko, Hulleman, and Harackiewicz (2011) proposed, mastery-approach goals may also predict higher academic performance, yet indirectly through some intervening mechanisms. Remarkably, although this suggestion seems to resolve several issues regarding whether mastery-approach goals can also predict higher performance, it has received little attention. This is unfortunate because if mastery-approach goals are indeed conducive of higher grades, through different channels, then their utility value can be further underscored.

Showing thus that mastery-approach goals can also predict academic performance, may render performance-approach goals even less attractive among teachers, parents, or education-policy makers who might favor them. In that way, students may be further discouraged to endorse performance-approach goals and thus they may be further protected from their side effects. In our study, we aimed to shed light on this issue. In particular, we aimed to investigate whether mastery-approach goals can also predict, next to performance-approach goals, higher grades, yet indirectly through challenge seeking. We opted for challenge seeking as it is considered a key marker of adaptive achievement striving (Dweck, 1986) and an index of students' cognitive, affective, and motivational growth (Meyer, Turner, & Spencer, 1997). By showing that mastery-approach goals may also predict higher grades at school, we aimed to further highlight the usefulness of mastery-approach goals as the safe route through which students can pursue their academic goals, including higher grades.

Broadly speaking, achievement goals have been defined as the reasons for which people strive for success in achievement settings (Ames and Archer, 1988, Dweck and Leggett, 1988, Nicholls, 1984). The most commonly agreed conceptualization of achievement goals distinguishes them depending on how people define and valence (i.e., appraise) competence (Elliot & McGregor, 2001). Based on that definition, three types of achievement goals seem especially relevant in the educational contexts; these are (a) mastery-approach goals when competence is defined with absolute or self-referenced criteria and is valenced positively; (b) performance-approach goals when competence is defined with normative criteria and is valenced positively; and (c) performance-avoidance goals when competence is defined with normative criteria and is valenced negatively. A student who strives to comprehend the subject material of the day or to improve her level of understanding is supposed to endorse mastery-approach goals, while a student who aims at outperforming her peers is supposed to pursue performance-approach goals. Accordingly, a student who aims at avoiding being worse than his or her classmates is said to adopt performance-avoidance goals.

Research has shown that mastery-approach goals are the most consistent and reliable predictors of a wide range of desired outcomes including, but not limited to, interest (Hulleman et al., 2010), enjoyment (Daniels et al., 2009), and challenge seeking (Lee & Kim, 2014). In contrast, performance-avoidance goals have been associated with undesired outcomes such as heightened anxiety (Daniels et al., 2009) and challenge avoidance (Jagacinski et al., 2008, Shim and Ryan, 2005). Performance-approach goals have shown a mixed pattern. Some studies for instance have found that they did not differ from mastery-approach goals in outcomes such as task involvement and enjoyment among achievement oriented people (Elliot & Harackiewicz, 1994). Other studies have also shown that although performance-approach goals are not associated with some positive outcomes such as intrinsic motivation (Murayama & Elliot, 2009) and challenge seeking (Lee & Kim, 2014), they predict academic performance (Barron and Harackiewicz, 2003, Wolters, 2004).

Regarding the main aim of our study, the relation of mastery-approach goals and performance-approach goals to school performance, recent literature has revealed that performance-approach goals that are defined as a pure aim to outperform others, rather than as an overarching reason to demonstrate high competence (Elliot, 2005; cf. Kaplan & Maehr, 2007), predict in a more reliable way academic performance, than mastery-approach goals (Bipp and van Dam, 2014, Durik et al., 2009, Elliot et al., 1999). These findings are consistent with the meta-analysis of Hulleman et al. (2010) but contradict a more recent meta-analysis which has shown that situationally induced mastery-approach goals, as compared to performance-approach ones, lead to somewhat better performance in verbal tasks (Van Yperen, Blaga, & Postmes, 2015).

Regarding school performance, there are also a few studies which have shown that school performance is predicted either by both mastery-approach and performance-approach goals (Chen, 2015, Church et al., 2001, Linnenbrink, 2005, Matos et al., 2017 - Sample 2; Niepel et al., 2014, Senko et al., 2013, Song et al., 2015) or by mastery-approach goals only (Keys et al., 2012, Lau and Nie, 2008, Matos et al., 2017 - Sample 1; Shim, Ryan, & Anderson, 2008). Yet, most of these studies showing mastery-approach goals being equivalent or superior to performance-approach goals relied on an earlier conceptualization of performance-approach goals according to which the aim of outperforming others is intertwined with ego concerns – for instance the higher-order aim to demonstrate superior ability (see also Kaplan & Maehr, 2007). Therefore, it remains unclear whether mastery-approach goals are more conducive than performance-approach goals when goals are devoid of a higher-order reason (e.g., to develop competence for mastery-approach goals; to demonstrate competence for performance-approach goals).

Indeed, research has shown that when performance-approach goals are operationally defined as pure aims (i.e., just to outperform others), they predict school performance in a more consistent way than mastery-approach goals do (Hulleman et al., 2010, Senko et al., 2011). Does this mean that students should favor performance-approach goals over mastery-approach goals if they are to achieve academically? Should they become more vigilant only on what is required to learn (Senko et al., 2013) thereby following the teachers' agenda (Hulleman & Senko, 2010)? Not at all, because mastery-approach goals may still lead to improved academic performance. But they may do so in an indirect way. In support of this view, a few studies with university students have shown that mastery-approach goals predict, next to performance-approach goals, higher grades through interest (Harackiewicz, Durik, Barron, Linnenbrink-Garcia, & Tauer, 2008) or enjoyment (Daniels et al., 2009).

Yet, as this brief overview of studies that operationally defined achievement goals as pure aims suggests, the indirect paths linking mastery-approach goals with academic performance have been shown in studies conducted in higher-education contexts and among university students. So, a question that awaits answering is whether a similar path exist in secondary educational systems, where interest, enjoyment, or challenge is less likely to manifest in courses, like mathematics, that are compulsory rather than elective. To the best of our knowledge, there has been only one study which investigated a similar process among high school students and which has also shown through path analysis mastery-approach goals to predict higher grades through interest (Dinger, Dickhauser, Spinath, & Steinmayr, 2013). Yet, in that research the effects of prior grades were not considered. We therefore intended to revisit this issue by controlling for prior grades, by assessing performance-approach goals as well as mastery-approach and performance-avoidance goals as pure aims, and by examining challenge seeking as a particular mechanism that mediates the relation between mastery-approach (but not performance-approach or performance-avoidance goals) and school performance.

As said, challenge seeking constitutes an adaptive motivational response pattern (Dweck & Leggett, 1988), namely because it is inherently tied with intrinsic motivation (Lepper, Corpus, & Iyengar, 2005). Indeed, students who enjoy challenges (or perceive academic tasks as such) are more likely to recruit their inner resources and invest more time and effort in their schoolwork (Putwain et al., 2016, Strati et al., 2017). Besides, as Grant and Dweck (2003) have shown, seeking challenging tasks and striving for learning are closely associated to each other and they together predict more energy expenditure and persistence in class work (see also Donnellan, 2008). Apparently, such an adaptive response pattern is presumed to facilitate performance in the long run (Dettmers, Trautwein, Lüdtke, Kunter, & Baumert, 2010) because, among others, challenge seeking seems to coincide with deep strategy use, preference for difficult tasks, and taking action in the face of impediments (Turner, Thorpe, & Meyer, 1998).

Further support to the view that challenge seeking may act as the link between mastery approach goals and school performance comes from the meta-analytic review conducted by Hulleman et al. (2010). These authors found that mastery-approach goals that in their operational definition embrace the notion of challenge seeking and (or) interest were more positively related to performance (r = 0.14) as compared to mastery-approach goals that focus on mastery and improvement (r = 0.05). This meta-analytic finding implies that challenge-seeking may perhaps drive part of the relation between mastery-approach goals and performance, something which becomes unnoticed once mastery-approach goals are defined, and assessed, as pure aims. That is, as goals simply aiming at mastering, learning, and understanding a task in hand (Elliot & Murayama, 2008). Even under such conceptualization however, mastery-approach goals may still relate to higher academic achievement because, as Senko et al. (2011) argue, mastery-approach goals may activate one's curiosity or challenge. Therefore, although it seems that challenge seeking may mediate, and explain, the relation between mastery-approach goals and academic performance, there is no empirical research testifying this proposition. The main aim of our study thus was to test this hypothesis. Furthermore, we investigated whether perceptions of classroom environment (namely, the degree to which classroom environment favors mastery strivings or competition) may also account for any variance in the endorsement of mastery-approach, performance-approach, and performance-avoidance goals.

In the achievement goal research tradition, the learning environment of a classroom, termed goal structures (Ames, 1992), is considered to direct students toward endorsing certain achievement goals (Meece et al., 2006, Meece et al., 2006). Perceived classroom goal structures have been usually discerned as those that promote learning, mastery, and improvement (and thus termed mastery goal structures), those that favor competition and rivalry among students (and thus termed performance-approach goal structures), and those that highlight avoiding doing mistakes and showing incompetence in front of the others (and thus termed performance-avoidance goal structures).

There is ample evidence in the literature to claim that mastery goal structures are linked with more desired outcomes than performance goal structures (for a review, see Rolland, 2012). For instance, perceived mastery goal structures have been associated positively with intrinsic motivation (Murayama & Elliot, 2009), help-seeking (Schenke, Lam, Conley, & Karabenick, 2015), cognitive strategies (Wolters, 2004), and academic achievement (Lau and Nie, 2008, Roseth et al., 2008), either directly, or indirectly through the endorsement of mastery-approach goals (Kaplan and Maehr, 2007, Wolters, 2004). Conversely, performance-approach goal structures have been associated negatively with certain desired outcomes such as intrinsic motivation (Murayama & Elliot, 2009), achievement, and task engagement and positively with some unwanted outcomes such as effort withdrawal and avoidance coping (Lau & Nie, 2008). Yet, with respect to achievement, a quasi-experimental study showed that students performed better when they were found in classrooms in which more emphasis was put on performance-approach goals or on performance-approach goals along with mastery-approach goals (Linnenbrink, 2005).

To summarize, the link between perceived goal structures and academic correlates has been shown through three different, sometimes overlapping, approaches. The first considers perceived goal structures, along with achievement goals, as antecedents of the school-related outcomes. The second approach presumes that goal structures enhance the adoption of certain achievement goals which in turn predict academic outcomes, whereas the third one investigates whether goal structures (namely as a classroom characteristic) moderate the association between personally endorsed achievement goals and outcomes (for an overview, see Murayama & Elliot, 2009). In this study, we also examined whether perceived goal structures would relate to grades through the endorsement of the respective achievement goals and, in turn, through challenge-seeking. In doing so, we aimed to contribute as well to the discussion of whether perceived goal structures predict educational outcomes because they favor certain achievement goals (Meece et al., 2006), without ignoring however the reciprocal relation between perceived goal structure and achievement goals (as one's personal achievement goals may lead one to pay more attention to those environmental cues that confirm and validate the pursuit of these goals) (Nickerson, 1998). We avoided examining whether goal structures would moderate the association between achievement goals and grades as such a test would have required multilevel analyses. Yet, this analysis was not feasible because of the limited number of available classes (Hox, 2010).

In this study, our main aim was to investigate to what extent academic achievement would be explained through two different channels - a direct one that entails endorsing performance-approach goals (something which has been already shown in prior research, yet mainly with university students), and an indirect one that involves challenge seeking as a mediating mechanism linking mastery-approach goals to academic achievement. We focused on challenge seeking as a likely mediator of the relation between mastery-approach goals and higher grades for three reasons. First, because challenge seeking is by itself a desired educational outcome that reflects adaptive motivational processes (Dweck, 1986). Second because several studies have shown that challenge seeking predict higher grades (Lepper et al., 2005). Third, because in the earlier conceptualization of mastery-approach goals where challenge seeking was inherently tied with mastery-approach goals as an underlying reason (e.g., “It is very important to me to feel that my coursework offers me real challenges”), mastery-approach goals were found to predict academic performance (Grant & Dweck, 2003; see also Hulleman et al., 2010).

To this direction and in line with the call of Senko et al. (2011), we hypothesized that challenge seeking would act as a mediator between mastery-approach goals and end-year grades even after we controlled for mid-semester grades (Hypothesis 1). Further, in line with the revised achievement goal theory which (a) operationally defines performance-approach goals as aims for surpassing others which are not necessarily tied with ego concerns (Elliot, 2005), and which (b) has shown that such kind of goals predict better academic performance (Hulleman et al., 2010), we anticipated performance-approach goals to directly predict higher grades, even after controlling for baseline grades (Hypothesis 2). Regarding performance-avoidance goals, we expected, in line with prior studies which have highlighted their maladaptive nature (Jagacinski et al., 2008, Shim and Ryan, 2005), to relate negatively to challenge seeking and grades (Hypothesis 3).

Moreover, we explored whether perceived goals structures would predict higher grades and challenge seeking either directly or indirectly through achievement goals (Research Question 1). Irrespective of whether achievement goals would fully, partly, or do not mediate the relation between perceived goal structures and educational outcomes, we anticipated perceived mastery goal structures to relate positively to mastery-approach goals (Hypothesis 4a) and perceived performance goal structures to relate positively to both performance-approach and performance-avoidance goals (Hypothesis 4b). This is because literature has indicated that the achievement goals that a teacher is perceived to emphasize in his or her classroom are more easily adopted by the students (Meece et al., 2006).

To examine our hypotheses, we recruited a sample of Turkish high school students and focused on the math class. In doing so, we could examine the associations among perceived goal structures, personal achievement goals, and grades in a hypercompetitive context, given that the Turkish educational system is normatively selective. Many Turkish middle school students aspire to achieve high to be admitted to renowned high schools (through national exams) and the same is true for high school students who aspire to enter University because tertiary education, as compared to non-tertiary education, offers much more job opportunities in Turkey (OECD, 2012). The math course was chosen as it is one of the main subject matters that students are tested to enter University.

This study was part of a larger longitudinal research project, funded by the National Research Council in Turkey (TUBITAK). The study was approved by the Turkish Ministry of Education and by the ethical review board of the host University and was in accordance with the ethical treatment of human subjects. An approval to visit the selected schools, all located in the district of Ankara, was granted by the Ministry of Education, the school principals and school counselors, and the board of

Preliminary analyses indicated significant differences in the linear combination of the measured variables as a function of gender (Wilk's Λ = 0.828, F[8, 277] = 7.17, p < 0.01, multivariate η2 = 0.17) and grade level (Wilk's Λ = 0.944, F[8, 277] = 2.06, p < 0.040), but not as a function of parents' education (Wilk's Λ = 0.937, F[16, 554] = 1.15, p = 0.31) or family income (Wilk's Λ = 0.951, F[16, 554] = 0.88, p = 0.59). Inspection of the bivariate correlations however suggested that three out of the four covariates

In this study we investigated, after controlling for mid-semester grades in mathematics (as well as students' gender, grade-level and their parents' education level and family income), whether end-year grades in that mathematics can be predicted through two distinct routes, the one entailing the promotion and endorsement of mastery-approach goals and the other involving the promotion and endorsement of performance-approach goals. Our findings indicate, in agreement with Senko et al. (2011),

From the achievement goal perspective, there seems to exist (at least) two different paths that may be associated with success: As our research suggests, though performance-approach goals seem to directly predict higher grades, mastery-approach goals can do so through challenge-seeking. Given that challenge seeking is a valuable outcome per se, and that perceived mastery goal structures, and in turn mastery goals endorsement, can also predict academic performance teachers will benefit students

We would like to thank the students for their participation as well and the principals the counselors and the teachers of the participating schools for their collaboration and support. We are also fully indebted to Aylin Koçak, Bariş Şahin, Özge Özgurt, and Bilgen Nur Yazici for their valuable contribution during data collection.

This work was supported by the Scientific and Technological Research Council of Turkey (TÜBITAK) [grant number 114K815].

  • A.S. Yeung et al.
  • C. Senko et al.
  • K. Schenke et al.
  • D.W. Putwain et al.
  • S.B. Nolen et al.
  • C. Niepel et al.
  • J.L. Meece et al.
  • L. Matos et al.
  • T.D. Keys et al.
  • A.M. Durik et al.

  • F.C. Dinger et al.
  • W.-W. Chen
  • T. Bipp et al.
  • K.E. Barron et al.
  • S. Abuhamdeh et al.
  • C. Ames
  • C. Ames et al.
  • L. Baker et al.
  • J. Brophy
  • M. Bugler et al.
  • M.A. Church et al.
  • L.M. Daniels et al.
  • C. Darnon et al.
  • S. Dettmers et al.
  • M.B. Donnellan
  • C.S. Dweck
  • C.S. Dweck et al.
  • A.J. Elliot
  • A.J. Elliot et al.
  • A.J. Elliot et al.
  • A.J. Elliot et al.
  • A.J. Elliot et al.
  • A.J. Elliot et al.
  • H. Grant et al.
  • R.K. Hambleton et al.
  • J.M. Harackiewicz et al.
  • J.M. Harackiewicz et al.
  • J.M. Harackiewicz et al.
  • A.F. Hayes
    • Research suggests that undergraduates' learning and performance-approach goals initiate distinct pathways to course achievement through different task values. However, few studies were longitudinal or controlled for initial task values. It therefore remains unclear whether task values mediate the relation between initial achievement goals and final grades beyond initial task values. The present study examined these pathways in two year-long college courses (N = 175). When initial task values were not controlled, the hypothesized pathways emerged: Semester 1 learning goals predicted year-end course grades via Semester 2 intrinsic value whereas Semester 1 performance-approach goals predicted course grades via Semester 2 attainment value. When Semester 1 values were controlled, however, achievement goals generally did not predict task values, although Semester 2 intrinsic and attainment value remained independent predictors of grades. These findings help to clarify and reconcile previous research findings and underscore the importance of accounting for construct stability to elucidate motivational processes.

    • Recent research anchored in achievement goal theory suggests mastery goals are more adaptive when endorsed for autonomous rather than controlled reasons. We report on two studies (N = 622) in which we explored whether the combined effects of goals and reasons on academic outcomes were different for a sample of low-SES youth than for other older higher-SES samples in the literature. Participants were low-SES high-school students in Lima, Peru. The results show that autonomous reasons for endorsing mastery goals positively predicted students' collective engagement and mathematics grades above the effect of mastery goals as such. Second, controlled reasons negatively predicted end-of-the year math grades. Finally, mastery goals’ relations with mathematics grades and behavioral engagement were attenuated when endorsed for low autonomous reasons. The findings extend the knowledge on mastery goal-complexes and show they apply to low-SES students.

    • This study examined associations between student- (N = 1414), classroom-, and teacher (N = 91) growth orientation and mathematics engagement and achievement using a multilevel model. Results demonstrated positive associations between student growth orientation and mathematics outcomes and positive associations between classroom and teacher growth orientation and mathematics achievement. There was also a marginally significant interaction effect between classroom and teacher growth orientation on classroom engagement. Findings hold relevance for educational interventions surrounding growth constructs and teachers’ personal growth orientation.

    • Academic growth constructs, such as growth mindset and various forms of growth goals, have been of substantial focus in psycho-educational research. Recent research has sought to identify how such growth constructs are inter-related, finding that an underlying growth orientation (comprised of growth mindset, self-based growth goals, and task-based growth goals) was cross-sectionally associated with more positive outcomes for students. However, for such a construct to have meaningful relevance to education and educational research, it must be associated with actual growth in academic outcomes. Accordingly, using two-wave longitudinal structural equation modeling in a large sample of Australian middle and high school students (N = 2949), we examined the extent to which students’ growth orientation predicted growth in academic outcomes. We hypothesized that students’ growth orientation would be positively associated with gains in students’ mathematics engagement and achievement, while controlling for student demographic covariates (e.g., gender, age) and prior variance in each substantive factor measured one year prior. Results demonstrated that students' growth orientation in mathematics was a significant positive predictor of students' gains in mathematics engagement and achievement, above other sources of influence. Findings are discussed in terms of improving researchers’ understanding of how growth constructs are inter-related and how to promote students’ academic success in mathematics.

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    • Although prior work has shown that mastery-approach achievement goals are related to positive learning behaviors (e.g., more interest, perseverance, and self-regulation), less is known about how these goals interact with instruction to influence knowledge transfer. To address these issues we conducted a laboratory experiment investigating how two aspects of the instructional environment, the task structure (tell-and-practice direct instruction vs. minimally-guided open-ended invention activities) and the task framing (mastery vs. performance), affected students' task-based mastery goal adoption and transfer when learning statistics. The results showed that structure was more effective than framing in manipulating students' mastery-approach goal adoption. In addition, students' existing mastery-approach orientations for mathematics strongly predicted knowledge transfer for all of the instructional conditions except for students given invention activities with a performance framing. For these students, the relationship between mastery-approach orientation and transfer was not observed, indicating that this condition makes transfer more likely for those lower in mastery-approach orientation. The results are discussed in terms of the implications for theories of achievement goal motivation, knowledge transfer, and instruction.

    • By using the 2 × 2 achievement goal framework, this study assessed the math achievement goal profiles of 488 Taiwanese students from Grades 7 to 8. In the first year, three math achievement goal profiles were identified using latent profile analysis: maladaptive, indifferent, and success-oriented. A slightly different profile, avoidant-but-adapting, was found in the second year in addition to the indifferent and success-oriented profiles. Success-oriented students showed the most adaptive pattern of motivation, with high approach orientations, while maladaptive students demonstrated less-adaptive learning characteristics, with high avoidance orientations. Avoidant-but-adapting students exhibited high scores for avoidance and mastery-approach orientations. The goal profiles were investigated with respect to stability and change over one year transition. Our study reflects mastery-avoidance goal's unique contribution to the classification of students and its' influences on educational and psychological outcomes and provides insights into the 2 × 2 model of achievement goal orientations.

    • This study examined the longitudinal reciprocal relations between academic self-concept, achievement goals (i.e., performance-approach, performance-avoidance, and mastery), and achievement (i.e., self-reported grades) in mathematics. The research aim was twofold. First, we examined the confound hypothesis, which states that performance-approach goals do not feature any incremental validity in predicting achievement over and above students' competence perceptions (i.e., academic self-concept). In addition, we expanded research on the confound hypothesis by also investigating performance-avoidance and mastery goals. Second, we investigated the predictive validity of all three achievement goals for changes in academic self-concept. Seven hundred sixty-nine students (50.78% female) attending the highest track of the German three-tier secondary school system participated in three waves of measurement in Grades 5, 6, and 8. Our findings confirmed the confound hypothesis: Performance-approach goals did not explain achievement over and above academic self-concept. The same findings applied to performance-avoidance and mastery goals. Furthermore, performance-approach goals were positively related to academic self-concept changes, whereas performance-avoidance goals showed a negative relation to academic self-concept changes over time. Mastery goals were not associated to changes in academic self-concept. Academic self-concept and achievement showed positive reciprocal relations. To conclude, our results point to complex relations between achievement goals, academic self-concept, and academic achievement over time.

    • In this paper we present the results of a study meant to investigate whether traditional and nontraditional students differ in their achievement goals as described by the 3 × 2 framework of Achievement Goal Theory. A total of 121 traditional and 36 nontraditional students were assessed using the 3 × 2 Achievement Goal Questionnaire. The traditional students endorsed other-approach and other-avoidance goals to a greater degree than nontraditional students. Other-avoidance goals were negatively related to students’ cumulative GPA. We discuss the implications of our findings concerning differences in traditional and nontraditional students’ achievement goals, along with avenues for future research.

    • The most surprising and controversial finding in achievement goal research is that performance goals predict academic achievement more reliably than do mastery goals. This paper reviews and tests multiple explanations for those effects. Each assumes that either goal can facilitate achievement under particular learning conditions. The challenge framework posits that performance goals facilitate achievement only on simple tasks, and that mastery goals do so on challenging tasks. The depth of learning framework posits that performance goals facilitate achievement only when assignments assess superficial topic knowledge, and that mastery goals do so when assignments assess deeper knowledge. The learning agenda framework posits that performance goals facilitate achievement only if task demands are clear, and that mastery goals can do so if students’ interests match the core topics assessed on assignments. In a test of these frameworks, university students from numerous courses reported their achievement goals and study strategies for an upcoming exam. Exams served as the measure of academic achievement and were also coded for features relevant to each framework’s hypothesized moderator variable. Analyses supported only the learning agenda framework. Discussion suggests additional ways to test each framework and considers implications for teachers and goal theorists.

    • We investigated the extent to which perceived structure and personal achievement goals could explain students' effective learning strategies and affect-related experiences in a sample of Greek adolescent students (N = 606; 45.4% males; mean age: M = 15.05, SD = 1.43). Having controlled for students' social desirability responses, we used multilevel analyses, and found that between-student (i.e., within class) differences in perceived structure related positively to learning strategies and positive affect and negatively to negative affect, with the relations being partially mediated by competence need satisfaction. In addition, we found between-student differences in the relations of mastery-approach, performance-approach, and performance-avoidance goals to the learning-strategy and affect outcomes. Moreover, at the between-class level, perceived structure related positively to learning strategies and positive affect, and negatively to depressive feelings. Finally, an interesting cross-level interaction between perceived structure and performance-avoidance goals for negative affect revealed that well-structured classrooms attenuated the positive, harmful relation between performance-avoidance goals and negative affect. These findings indicate the key role of structure and the endorsement of mastery-approach goals in the classroom.

    View full text